WELCOME to The Catalyst!
Sidebar

PHYSICS

REFERENCES:

R. Baillargeon, E. Spelke, and S. Wasserman, (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191-208.

R. Baillargeon, (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: objects permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21-41.

F. Bueche. (1988). Principles of Physics. McGraw Hill.

M. Bullock, R. Gelman, and R. Barllargeon, (1982). The development of causal reasoning. The Developmental Psychology of Time, Academic Press, Inc.

A. Caramazza, M. McCloskey, and B. Green, (1981). Naive beliefs in "sophisticated" subjects: Misconceptions about trajectories of objects. Cognition, 1981, 9, 117-123.

R. Catrambone, C. Jones, J. Jonides, and C. Seifert, (1995). Reasoning about curvilinear motion: using principles or analogy. Memory and Cognition, 23(3), 368-373.

J. Clement, (1982). Students' preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of Physics, 50(1), 66-71.

J. Clement, (1983). A conceptual model discussed by Galileo and used intuitively by physics students. In D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

A. Damasio, (1994). Descartes' Error. NY: Grosset/Putnam.

M. Delbruck, (1986). Mind from matter. Blackwell Scientific Publications.

A. diSessa, (1979). in Cognitive Process. J. Lockhead and J. Clement (eds.). Frankin Inst.

A. diSessa, (1983). Phenomenology and the evolution of physics. In D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

A. diSessa, (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105-225.

R. Driver, (1973). Ph.D Dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champain.

L. Franks, (1969). Signal Theory. NJ: Prentice-Hall.

J. Freyd, (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, Vol. 94, No. 4, 427-438.

J. Freyd, T. Pantzer, and J. Cheng, (1988). Representing static as forces in equilibrium. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 117, No. 4, 395-407.

J. Gibson, (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

D. Gilden and D. Proffitt, (1989). Understanding Collision Dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Vol. 15, No. 2, 372-383.

D. Gilden, (1991). On the origins of dynamic awareness. Psychological Review, Vol. 98, No. 4, 554-568.

A. Halloun and D. Hestenes, (1985). The initial knowledge state of college physics students. American Journal of Physics, 53(11), 1043-1055.

A. Halloun and D. Hestenes, (1985). Common sense concepts about motion. American Journal of Physics, 53(11), 1056-1065.

N. Humphrey, (1992). A history of mind. NY: Simon & Schuster.

S. Hykin, (1986). Adaptive Filter Theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.

M. Jammer, (1957). Concepts of force: a study of dynamics. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.

M. Jammer, (1961). Concepts of mass, in classical and modern physics. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.

G. Johansson, C. Hofsten, and G. Jansson, (1980). Event perception. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 31, 27-63.

M. Kaiser and D. Proffitt, (1984). The development of sensitivity to causally relevant dynamic information. Child Development, 55, 1614-1624.

M. Kaiser, J. Jonides, and J. Alexander, (1986). Intuitive reasoning about abstract and familiar physics problems. Memory and Cognition, 14(4), 308-312.

M. Kaiser, M. McCloskey, and D. Proffit, (1986). Development if intuitive theories of motion: curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces. Developmental Psychology, Vol. 22, No. 1, 67-71.

M. Kaiser and D. Proffitt, (1987). Observers's sensitivity to dynamic anomalies in collisions. Perception and Psychophysics, 42(3), 275-280.

M. Kaiser, D. Proffitt, S. Whelan, and H. Hecht, (1992). Influence of animation on dynamic judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performances, Vol. 18, No. 3, 669-690.

I. Kant, (1990). Critique of Pure Reason. New York: Prometheus.

M. Leyton, (1992). Symmetry, Causality, and Mind. MIT Press.

I. Leclerc, (1972). The nature of physical existence. New York: Humanities press.

A. Leslie and S. Keeble, (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265-288.

A. Leslie, (1988). The necessity of illusion: perception and thought in infancy. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought Without Language, Oxford: Oxford Science Publications.

A. Leslie, (1993). A theory of agency. Technical Report, Rutgers University, Center for Cognitive Science. TR-12.

A. Leslie, (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: core architecture and domain specificity. In L. Hirschfeld and S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, Cambridge University Press.

L. McDermott, (1984). Research on conceptual understanding in mechanic. Physics Today, 24-32.

M. McCloskey, A. Caramazza, and B. Green, (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: naive beliefs about the motion of objects. Science, 1980, 210, 1139-1141.

M. McCloskey, (1983). Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

A. Michotte, (1963). The perception of causality. Andover: Methuen.

J. Minstrell, (1982). Explaining the 'at rest' condition of an object. The physics Teacher, 10-14.

T. Natsoulas, (1960). Judgments of velocity and weight in a causal situation. American Journal of Psychology, 73, 404-410.

T. Natsoulas, (1961). Principles of momentum and kinetic energy in the perception of causality. American Journal of Psychology, 74, 394--402.

A. Oppenheim and R. Schafer, (1975). Digital Signal Processing, Prentice-Hall, EnglewoodCliffs, N.J.

J. Pittenger, (1983). Cognitive physics and event perception: two approaches to the assessment of people's knowledge of physics. In D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

J. Pittenger, (1990). Detection of violations of the law of pendulum motion: observers' sensitivity to the relation between period and length. Ecological Psychology, 2(1), 55-81.

D. Proffitt and D. Gilden, (1989). Understanding Natural Dynamics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performances, Vol. 15, No. 2, 384-393.

P. Roland and E. Mortensen, (1987). Somatosensory detection of microgeometry, macrogeometry and kinesthesia in man. Brain Research Reviews, 12, 1-42.

S. Roncato and R. Rumiati, (1986). Naive statics: current misconceptions on equilibrium.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 12, No. 3, 361-377.

S. Runeson and G. Frykholm, (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Geneal, Vol. 112, No.4 585-615.

C. E. Shanon, (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J., 27:379-423, 623-656.

E. Spelke, K. Breinlinger, J. Macomber, and K. Jacobson, (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, Vol. 99, No. 4, 605-632.

E. Spelke, (1988). Origins of physical knowledge. In L Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought Without Language, Oxford: Oxford Science Publications.

L. Talmy, (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49-100.

J. Todd and W. Warren, (1982). Visual perception of relative mass in dynamic events. Perception, Vol. 11, 325-335.

F. Varela, E. Thompson, and E. Rosch, (1991). The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

L. Viennot, (1979). European J. of Science Education. 1. 205.

B. White, (1983). Sources of difficulty in understanding Newtonian dynamics. Cognitive Science, 7, 41-65.

J. Yates, (1985). The content of awareness is a model of the world. Psychological Review, 92, 249-284.

J. Yates, (1988). Are conceptions of motion based on a naive theory or on prototypes. Cognition, 29, 251-275.

Physics